When reading with a student, which matters more
– the amount of support you give the child when they encounter a difficult
word, or the type of support you give?
The answer might surprise you. In an article in the March/April issue of The Reading Teacher, Emily Rodgers describes
her study of 10 Reading Recovery teachers and the level and type of scaffolding
they provided students as they read. They separated the teachers into two
groups – teachers who tended to have results above the national average for
Reading Recovery and teachers who tended to have results below the national
average. To assess the level of support
they evaluated teachers’ use of Wood’s (2003) tutoring rule: “When the learner
runs into difficulty, the teacher should increase the amount of help provided,
and when the learner experiences, success, the teacher should decrease the
amount of help” (Rodgers, 2017, p. 527).
Rodgers found that there was no significant difference between the two
groups of teachers. All teachers adjusted the level of their instruction
appropriately about 61% of the time.
Rodgers did, however, find a very significant
difference between the two groups of teachers regarding type of support they
provided. Students (and all readers, for that matter) use three cueing systems
as they read: meaning, structure, and visual cues .
Proficient readers simultaneously use multiple cues, while struggling readers
tend to overuse one or two cues, often due to the instruction they’ve received
(Schwartz, 2005).
Rodgers found that high-performing teachers were
eight times more likely to intentionally
vary the type of support they gave in response to students by prompting them to
use the missing cueing system. In other words, if a student over-relied on
meaning cues (by misreading pony for horse or swing for playground)
then these teachers prompted them to use more visual cues. If the reader
overused visual cues (by saying visually similar nonsense words such as payund
for playground, for instance) then the teachers prompted them to attend
to meaning. This attention to the readers’ MSV errors and intentionality about
their teaching response resulted in much higher success for students.
It seems simple, right? Simply notice what the
student is not doing, and prompt them to do it. But Rodgers cautions that we
tend to find our “favorite” prompts and reuse them over and over again without
regard to the child in front of us.
She also cautions that just because there wasn’t
a significant difference between the amount of support provided by the two
groups of teachers doesn’t mean that we should stop adjusting how much support
we give. All students in the study made some progress, and that could have been
due to the teachers’ varying amount of support. Of course, if we don’t vary how
much support we give, we will likely cause frustration in the student, which
can be detrimental.
I found this interesting, because I’ve written before about amount and type of teacher
scaffolding, but this study forwards the notion that tailoring our prompts for
students to the quality of their miscues is by far the more important to attend
to.
No comments:
Post a Comment